Daw et al ’s finding that different subjects employ each system t

Daw et al.’s finding that different subjects employ each system to a greater or lesser degree (Daw et al., 2011) might be seen as being evidence for the latter idea. Various suggestions have been made for how arbitration should proceed, but this is an area where much more work is necessary. One idea is that it should depend on the relative uncertainties of the systems, trading the noise induced by the calculational difficulties of model-based control off against the noise

induced by the sloth of learning of model-free control (Daw et al., Selleckchem PFI-2 2005). This provides a natural account of the emergence of habitual behavior (Dickinson, 1985), as in the latter noise decreases as knowledge accumulates. By this account, it could be the continual uncertainty induced by the changing mazes in Simon and Daw (2011) that led to the persistent dominance of model-based control.

Equally, the uncertainty associated with unforeseen circumstances might lead to the renewed dominance of model-based control, even after model-free control had asserted itself (Isoda and Hikosaka, 2011). A different idea suggested by Keramati et al. (2011) starts from the observation that model-free values are fast to compute but potentially inaccurate, whereas model-based ones are slow to compute but typically more accurate (Keramati et al., 2011). They consider a regime in which the model-based Selleck ERK inhibitor values are essentially perfect and then perform a cost/benefit analysis to assess whether the value of this perfect information is sufficient to make it worth acquiring expensively. The model-free controller’s uncertainty about

the relative values of the action becomes a measure of the potential benefit; and the opportunity cost of the time of calculation (quantified by the prevailing average reward rate (Niv et al., 2007) is a measure of the cost. A related suggestion involves integration of model-free and model-based values rather than selection and a different method of model-based calculation (Pezzulo et al., 2013). There is no unique form of model-free or model-based control and evidence hints that there are intermediate points on the spectrum between them. For instance, there are important differences between model-free control based on the predicted Casein kinase 1 long-run values of actions (as in Q-learning) (Watkins, 1989), or SARSA (Rummery and Niranjan, 1994), and actor-critic control (Barto et al., 1983). In the latter, for which there is some interesting evidence (Li and Daw, 2011), action choice is based on propensities that convey strictly less information than the long-run values of those actions. There are even ideas that the spiraling connections between the striatum and the dopamine system (Joel and Weiner, 2000 and Haber et al., 2000) could allow different forms of controller to be represented in different regions (Haruno and Kawato, 2006).

Comments are closed.