In order
for the battery to be considered a good measure of general intelligence, this higher-order component should correlate with “g” as measured by a classical IQ test. The results presented here suggest that such higher-order constructs should be used with caution. On the one hand, a higher-order component may be used to generate a more interpretable first-order factor solution, for example, when cognitive tasks load heavily on multiple components. On the other hand, the basis of the higher-order component is ambiguous and may be accounted for by cognitive tasks corecruiting multiple functionally dissociable brain networks. Consequently, to interpret a higher-order component as representing a dominant unitary factor is misleading. Nonetheless, one potential objection to the results of the current study could be that while the 12 tasks load on common behavioral components, by this website Y-27632 cost the most commonly applied definition, these components do not relate to general intelligence unless they generate a second-order component that correlates with “g.” From this perspective, only the higher-order component may truly be considered intelligence, with the first-order components being
task specific. In the current study, this objection is implausible for several reasons. First, a cognitive factor that does not relate to such general processes as planning, reasoning, attention, and short-term memory would, by any sensible definition, be a very poor candidate for general intelligence. Furthermore, many of the tasks applied here were based on paradigms that either have been previously associated with general intelligence or form part of classical intelligence testing batteries. In line with this view, analysis of data from our pilot study shows that when a second-order component is generated, it correlates significantly with “g,” and yet, based on the imaging data,
that higher-order component is greatly reduced, as it may primarily be accounted for by tasks corecruiting multiple functionally dissociable brain networks. Moreover, MD cortex, which is both active during and necessary for the performance of classic intelligence tests, tuclazepam was highly activated during the performance of this cognitive battery but was divided into two functional networks. Thus, the tasks applied here both recruited and functionally fractionated the previously identified neural correlates of “g.” It should also be noted that this battery of tasks is, if anything, more diverse than those applied in classical IQ tests and, in that respect, may be considered at least as able to capture general components that contribute to a wide range of tasks. For example, Raven’s matrices (Raven, 1938) employ variants on one class of abstract reasoning problem, the Cattell uses just four types of problem, while the WAIS-R (Weschler, 1981) employs 11 subtests. Thus, it is clearly the case that by either definition, the tasks applied here are related to general intelligence.